Search the KHIT Blog

Wednesday, July 30, 2025

"You white people

all think that Caitlin Clark invented basketball."
 
 
I bought Christine Brennan's new book. Posted about it on BlueSky. Within mere minutes I got hammered with angry "you white people" stuff like the post headline above.
 
"deboraho" cracked on me and then blocked me.
 

Wasn't ready for that. I tersely repled "spare me." Whatever. I reciprocated the block. No point in engaging and escalating.
 
Yeah, right, "You White People." Pleeeze...
 
July 31 Update: I just finished. It is one fine book. 
 

 
More to come...

Tuesday, July 29, 2025

The Incumbent U.S. President:

“It would be nice to have at least a ‘thank you’.”

Saturday, July 26, 2025

ASI?

"Artificial SuperIntelligence?"
 
Click to enlarge.
On deck. No mention of the relatively pedestrian "IA" (Intelligence Augmentation).  Marketing hype conflation? And, no, it's not properly "Intelligent Automation," though the shifting hype terms and phrases seem to be measured in Scaramuccis these days.
 
While Artificial Intelligence-ish topics have been quite the hype rage of late, an exponential ramp-up may be on the cusp of unprecedented investment. Much of it underwritten opaquely by the Trump administration (w/scant regulation). Sam Altman and Zuck are vying intensely to be Donald's new BFF post-Elon. 
 
 
 
SOME TOPICALLY RELEVANT PRIOR POSTS 

Friday, July 25, 2025

Define "ignorance"

 
The Johnny Appleseed of ignorance.
“Ignorance is death” — could there be a better description of U.S. President Donald Trump?

Ignorance is Trump’s stock and trade, his life’s mission. He luxuriates in it and wears it as a crown. As a lover of ignorance, he surrounds himself with fellow travelers. He picked a Fox News blowhard as Secretary of Defense, an anti-vaxxer nutcase as his Secretary of Health and Human Services, and a totally unqualified sycophant as Director of National Intelligence. In short, some of the government’s top officials charged with the health and defense of the American people are totally unqualified.

As they say, what could go wrong?

At the same time, Trump empowered Elon Musk and his band of spoiled children to begin mass terminations of federal employees.

Moving at the speed of light, using both terminations and pressured early retirements, they forced people out by the thousands, often without the slightest idea of what job they did or what disruptions their departures might cause. And surprise — almost immediately agencies started asking some of these terminated, but indispensable, civil servants to return to their jobs.

As much as Trump's ignorance in running the government has harmed this country, the worst part isn’t his own incompetence at governing. It is the way he has managed, with the help of the far-right machine, to reengineer the thinking of millions of people to view ignorance as a virtue.

Under this “through the looking glass” view of the world, shooting from the hip is better than expertise and careful reflection. And this extends to the belief that one’s own gut feeling on scientific subjects, such as global warming and vaccinations, is more reliable than multiple studies produced under the scientific method.

Trump is the Johnny Appleseed of ignorance. It sprouts wherever he goes.

And this ignorance has proven to be deadly. The best-known example, of course, has been his response to the COVID-19 pandemic during its earlier, more deadly, phase. From the beginning, Trump did everything he could to minimize# the danger and spread conspiracy theories. He advocated quack remedies, discouraged mask wearing, and undermined scientific evidence. According to a study in The Lancet, Trumps actions during the early years of the pandemic caused hundreds of thousands of unnecessary deaths...

For many of these victims, trusting Trump was their last mistake…
 
Steven Day, Esq practices law in Wichita, KS and is a contributing writer at Common Dreams.  He is the author of the novel The Patriot's Grill.
 
"It is 2099 and America has been controlled by a brutal dictatorship for 70 years. Democracy isn’t just dead; it’s been erased from history. For Joe Carlton, bartender at The Patriot’s Grill, concepts of self-government and individual liberty are unimaginable. But then an old man, with an unbelievable story, wanders into the Grill and everything changes — for Joe and for the nation."
"IGNORANCE?"
 
Not a synonym for "stupid," notwithstanding its common use as an epithet implying that. In Donald Trump's case, the phrase "willful ignorance" comes straight to mind, connoting a too-much-trouble indifference to acquisition and retention of knowledge—Donald the Fabulist (see On Bullshit).
 
 
The shameless Trumpian fabulism has been on full display this week.
 
A relevant prior post, The Death of Truth
 
January 20th, 2017, The Presidential Oaf of Office
 
UPDATE
 
Shall we rewind the video a decade?
 
"Look, having nuclear—my uncle was a great professor and scientist and engineer, Dr. John Trump at MIT; good genes, very good genes, OK, very smart, the Wharton School of Finance, very good, very smart—you know, if you're a conservative Republican, if I were a liberal, if, like, OK, if I ran as a liberal Democrat, they would say I'm one of the smartest people anywhere in the world-it's true!- but when you're a conservative Republican they try—oh, do they do a number-that's why I always start off: Went to Wharton, was a good student, went there, went there, did this, built a fortune-you know I have to give my like credentials all the time, because we're a little disadvantaged-but you look at the nuclear deal, the thing that really bothers me it would have been so easy, and it's not as important as these lives are (nuclear is powerful; my uncle explained that to me many, many years ago, the power and that was 35 years ago; he would explain the power of what's going to happen and he was right-who would have thought?), but when you look at what's going on with the four prisoners—-now it used to be three, now it's four—but when it was three and even now, I would have said it's all in the messenger; fellas, and it is fellas because, you know, they don't, they haven't figured that the women are smarter right now than the men, so, you know, it's gonna take them about another 150 years—but the Persians are great negotiators, the Iranians are great negotiators, so, and they, they just killed, they just killed us."
The OAF of Office...
 

Thursday, July 24, 2025

They come at night, faces masked, black helmets, tactical gear, assault weaponry, no names, no insignia...

Donald Trump's Secret Police
Given Trump's Jeffrey Epstein CusterFluck of late, this extraconstitutional ICE/CBP deportation hardball has been way out of media headlines this week. These operatives have increasingly taken to violently arresting bystanders who try to video their raids with smartphones.

Tuesday, July 22, 2025

25th Amendment, anyone?

Donald Trump live on CNN in the White House taking press questions during his meeting with the President of the Philippines, July 22nd, 2025, ~12:03 pm.

0:00 … And you know, they went into him very, you know, in great detail. They gave me the whole thing. And what can I do? They say that it was just a nut job that was looking to do this. And I I spoke with the FBI, the new FBI. I spoke to FBI. If it was the old FBI, I wouldn't have believed a thing they said because the old FBI under Comey was crooked as hell. He was one of the most crooked. Everybody should read the Horowitz report. Unfortunately, Bill Barr didn’t use it, but the Horowitz report, not appointed by me, it was a report on Comey and the FBI. It is one of the worst uh shots at a human being I think I've ever seen. In fact, the New York Times did an editorial that was one of the worst editorials I've ever seen about anybody. That was about Comey. You had to go back and get and Barr didn't use it because he didn't have the guts to use it or something happened. I don't know. Too bad. He went to Mr. Durham instead of you instead of doing the Durham all they had to use is the Harowitz report and I always felt badly for Harowitz frankly because uh he's a Democrat I guess he was appointed by a Democrat but that report has gotten lost and it shouldn't be lost. You should all go back. It should be mandatory reading. Go back and read the Horowits report on Comey and his cronies and you'll see exactly we're gonna add that to all the stuff that we found. It just confirms it. But what we found is even more so. Now, we found absolute This isn't like evidence or this is like proof, irrefutable proof that Obama was sedacious, that Obama led was trying to lead a coup. And it was with Hillary Clinton, with all these other people, but Obama headed it up. And it, you know, I I get a kick when I hear everyone talks about people I never even heard of. It was this one. No, no, it was Obama. He headed it up. And it says so right in the papers. Got everything Got everything. This is the biggest scandal in the history of our country And it really goes on to even the autopen because it all relates to the same thing. It all started the same sick minds. You have an autopen which is a tremendous well. We don't have a president. We have an autopen that signs everything. I'll bet you sign everything. You don't have auto pens. Everybody. Who doesn't sign? You're president of a country. But but it's all that whole thing leading right up to the to the end of it is the autopen. And the autopen was used by people and Biden knew nothing about it. Biden knew nothing about it. They were signing documents that he knew nothing about. As an example, they released the unselect committee of liability. The unselect committee spent two years grilling everybody and then they destroyed all the evidence. You know why? Because the evidence made them guilty. and he gave them all a pardon. The Cheney, all the all the scum that was on the unselect committee, Cryan, Adam Kinzinger and Democrats, and he gave them a pardon. Do you have something else? Go ahead… 3:12


The incumbent U.S. President just openly accused former President Obama of being summarily guilty of Treason on live national TV.
 
This is infuriatingly inept, glaringly dishonest, and way beneath the dignity of the office.
 

Monday, July 21, 2025

"On average, generative AIs write in more fluent language than most humans and produce very convincing deepfakes."

"The fluency heuristic that gets triggered is our unconscious tendency to accept material that is fluently presented regardless of its validity."
 
OK, back to Dek.ai for a bit.
 
 
Very interesting to me, for numerous reasons. 
I was an unremarkable "B" student in high school (1960-64). Opted to not attend college and instead went out on the road as a guitar player in a bar band.

Sixteen years later I entered undergraduate school at Tennessee. Divorced, custody of my two daughters, nothing to put in a resume.Time to make a substantive change.

Upon enrollment I took the CLEP Exam (College Level English Proficiency), a lengthy multiple-choice "reading comprehension / vocabulary" diagnostic with an essay section (topic unknown to the examinee ahead of time). At the time (1980), the national 99% percentile was 920.

I scored a 965.

That simply had to be a reflection of my compulsive reading habit, which had gotten fully underway by 1967, when I arrived in San Francisco and became fully politically aware. Didn't hurt that I lived in North Beach a few blocks west of the venerable City Lights Bookstore.

Across my adult life since that time I have continued to read 2-3 books a week on average, a broad range of periodicals, and, increasingly, all of the internet-based written material now widely available to all of us (i.e, our LLM "training data").

My AI/LLM joke now is that "I'm a Carbon-Based Relatively Large Recursive Language Model," a "CBRLRLM." A one-man "Fluency Heuristic," I suppose.

Got my undergrad at 39 and moved on to the white collar world.

My initial undergrad goal was to obtain a degree in advertising. I'd become a fairly adept photographer and learned some basics in print layout technique from a colleague guitarist who also had a degree in architecture from Auburn. I read everything I could find on the topics at the Knox County library.

My precise interest was focused on "B2B," corporate /industrial communications. While I enjoyed courses in copywriting, ad design, commercial TV production, etc, the UTK Ad Department was totally consumer products and services oriented. I had little enthusiasm for selling shampoo, cars, food & beverage, or tobacco products.

Coursework in deductive logic, inductive logic ("lying with statistics" according to my prof), philospohy of science, the gamut of stats classes, and the breadth of psychology curricula diverted my attention, with an eventual concerted study in "psychometrics" (psychological tests and measures design and empirical validation).

Then in January 1986 I got my first day gig. Systems programmer and QC analyst in a radiation lab in Oak Ridge.

Go figure.

I eventually engaged in other technical writing fields...
The training of a CBRLRLM 
 
More shortly. Stay tuned...

Saturday, July 19, 2025

"Trajector(ies)?"

AGI? Transhumanist evolution?
 
 
Just randoomly ran across this bright young fellow He's posted a lot of work. Interesting, in light of my recent posts on AI stuff like the DeKai Wu Raising AI book and similar things.
 
Then I got onto this via a Substack post. 
Why Science Hasn’t Solved Consciousness (Yet)
To understand life, we must stop treating organisms like machines and minds like code.


Much of our current discussion about consciousness has a singular fatal flaw. It’s a mistake built into the very foundations of how we view science — and how science itself is perceived and conducted across disciplines, including today’s hype around artificial intelligence.
Great piece. Includes an AI audio narration, ~22 min). Well worth your time. BTW: He had me at Shannon Vallor.

Well, then that essay.led me here:
 
… Our scientific worldview has gotten stuck in an impossible contradiction, making our present crisis fundamentally a crisis of meaning. On the one hand, science appears to make human life seem ultimately insignificant. The grand narratives of cosmology and evolution present us as a tiny contingent accident in a vast indifferent universe. On the other hand, science repeatedly shows us that our human situation is inescapable when we search for objective truth because we cannot step outside our human form and attain a God’s-eye view of reality. Cosmology tells us that we can know the universe and its origin only from our inside position, not from the outside. We live within a causal bubble of information—the distance light traveled since the big bang—and we cannot know what lies outside. Quantum physics suggests that the nature of subatomic matter cannot be separated from our methods of questioning and investigating it. In biology, the origin and nature of life and sentience remain a mystery despite marvelous advances in genetics, molecular evolution, and developmental biology. Ultimately, we cannot forgo relying on our own experience of being alive when we seek to comprehend the phenomenon of life. Cognitive neuroscience drives the point home by indicating that we cannot fully fathom consciousness without experiencing it from within. Each of these fields ultimately runs aground on its own paradoxes of inner versus outer, and observer versus observed, that collectively turn on the conundrum of how to understand awareness and subjectivity in a universe that was supposed to be fully describable in objective scientific terms without reference to the mind. The striking paradox is that science tells us both that we’re peripheral in the cosmic scheme of things and that we’re central to the reality we uncover. Unless we understand how this paradox arises and what it means, we’ll never be able to understand science as a human activity and we’ll keep defaulting to a view of nature as something to gain mastery over. 

Each of the cases just mentioned—cosmology and the origin of the universe, quantum physics and the nature of matter, biology and the nature of life, cognitive neuroscience and the nature of consciousness—represents more than an individual scientific field. Collectively they represent our culture’s grand scientific narratives about the origin and structure of the universe and the nature of life and the mind. They underpin the ongoing project of a global scientific civilization. They constitute a modern form of mythos: they are the stories that orient us and structure our understanding of the world. For these reasons, the paradoxes these fields face are more than mere intellectual or theoretical puzzles. They signal the larger unreconciled perspectives of the knower and the known, mind and nature, subjectivity and objectivity, whose fracture menaces our project of civilization altogether. Our present-day technologies, which drive us ever closer to existential threats, concretize this split by treating everything—including, paradoxically, awareness and knowing themselves—as an objectifiable, informational quantity or resource. It’s precisely this split—the divorce between knower and known and the suppression of the knower in favor of the known—that constitutes our meaning crisis. The climate emergency, which arises from our treating nature as just a resource for our use, is the most pronounced and catastrophic manifestation of our crisis. 

In short, although we have created the most powerful and successful form of objective knowledge of all time, we lack a comparable understanding of ourselves as knowers. We have the best maps we’ve ever made, but we’ve forgotten to take account of the map makers. Unless we change how we navigate, we’re bound to head deeper into peril and confusion.

Regrettably, the three best-known responses to the crisis of meaning in our scientific worldview are all dead ends. 

First, scientific triumphalism doubles down on the absolute supremacy of science. It holds that no question or problem is beyond the reach of scientific discourse. It advertises itself as the direct heir of the Enlightenment. But it simplifies and distorts Enlightenment thinkers who were often skeptical about progress and who had subtle and sophisticated views about the limits of science. Triumphalism’s conception of science remains narrow and outmoded. It leans heavily on problematic versions of reductionism—the idea that complex phenomena can always be exhaustively explained in terms of simpler phenomena—and crude forms of realism—the idea that science provides a literally true account of how reality is in itself apart from our cognitive interactions with it. Its view of objectivity rests on an often unacknowledged metaphysics of a perfectly knowable, definite reality existing “out there,” independent of our minds and actions. It often denies the value of philosophy and holds that more of the same narrow and outmoded thinking will show us the way forward. As a result, theoretical models become ever more contrived and distant from empirical data, while experimental resources are applied to low-risk research projects that eschew more fundamental questions. Like Victorian-era spiritualism and pining for ghosts, scientific triumphalism looks backward to the fantasized spirit of a long-dead age and cannot hope to provide a path forward through the monumental challenges that science and civilization face in the twenty-first century. 

Science denial on the right and so-called postmodernism on the left represent a second response. These movements reject science. They particularly reject its capacity to establish truths about the world that can be used as a basis for further knowledge and wise policies and actions. Worse still, they provide an opportunity for certain groups to manipulate how facts are interpreted for their own selfish and ideological ends, thereby spreading intentional disinformation in the form of so-called alternative facts and alternative truths. Although the motives of these two movements undeniably differ, both undermine the values of modern society on which they themselves depend, offering nothing but skepticism and negativism, or willful disinformation, in return. 

Finally, the new age movement uses fringe science or pseudoscience to justify wishful thinking. Although this movement has had less impact than the other ones, it has muddied the waters for those of us who look for new perspectives on the scientific endeavor. Its uncritical embrace of various misrepresentations of Asian or Indigenous worldviews takes reductionistic science as the norm of all science, and thereby fails to understand the scientific ideas and practices of these other cultures. As a result, constructive dialogue with other cultural traditions about their epistemic practices becomes rare, if not impossible. 

Given that these three responses fail to address the crisis of meaning in our scientific worldview, how can we find our way forward? First and foremost, we need to know where the crisis comes from. Our goal is to identify the source of the crisis, offer clues to a new path forward, and present a new perspective on some of the biggest issues science faces today. These issues include time and cosmology, quantum physics and its measurement problem, the nature of life and sentience, how the mind works and its relation to AI, the nature of consciousness, and, finally, climate change and Earth’s entry into the new human-shaped epoch called the Anthropocene. The range of topics touched by our new perspective is indeed broad, and the vista is equally expansive. We believe that our perspective can help transform and revive our cherished scientific culture as it faces its greatest challenges while reshaping our worldview for a sustainable project of civilization. 

We call the source of the meaning crisis the Blind Spot. At the heart of science lies something we do not see that makes science possible, just as the blind spot lies at the heart of our visual field and makes seeing possible. In the visual blind spot sits the optic nerve; in the scientific blind spot sits direct experience—that by which anything appears, shows up, or becomes available to us. It is a precondition of observation, investigation, exploration, measurement, and justification. Things appear and become available thanks to our bodies and their feeling and perceiving capacities. Direct experience is bodily experience. “The body is the vehicle of being in the world,” says French philosopher Maurice Merleau-Ponty, but as we will see, firsthand bodily experience lies hidden in the Blind Spot.

Frank, Adam; Gleiser, Marcelo; Thompson, Evan (2024). The Blind Spot: Why Science Cannot Ignore Human Experience (Introduction). Kindle Edition.
So many smart people. 
 
REGARDING EVOLUTION AND "TRANSHUMANISM."

... Intelligence begins with the sensitivity of a single cell, and by a process of biological accumulation and selection, transmitted genetically, reaches a current culmination in the complexity of the human brain. In the same way, the first chipped flint, by a process of cultural accumulation of knowledge passed on verbally and subjected to the selection of experience, reaches a current culmination in spacecraft, cyclotrons, and satellite-relayed television. At the same time, the natural and necessary playfulness of the young mammalian as it explores its environment and learns how to live in it, over time, and also by a cultural process of imitation, memory, and the transmission of knowledge and skills from generation to generation, reaches another apotheosis in the high cultures and great arts.

For the comparatively weak creature who must cope with an environment and rise to any situation or perish, the resources of mental equipment are applied first to the most urgent exigencies. The earliest application of intelligence is devoted to devising tools as weapons; those that follow aid the amenities of life; finally, religion, philosophy, and the arts make their appearance.

The kind of tools that are made will depend on the material that happens to be available, be it leaves, branches, stones, clay, or the presence or absence of minerals. Thus an interaction exists between environment and creature that affects the development of tools and culture as well as of senses and intelligence.

In ancient Egypt, for example, the presence of the papyrus plant led to the fabrication of ropes, mats, sandals, and eventually paper; the presence of flax made possible the eventual perfecting of supremely fine linens. In Central America the presence of lava led to cutting tools of obsidian. Once a cultural habit becomes established, however, it remains impossible to predict either the route it will follow or its ultimate outcome. Who could have foretold that the sweet potato-washing habit established among the members of the famous Japanese macaque colony, and the subsequent sifting of sand from grain in the sea, would so accustom these forest animals to playing in the water that they would eventually begin to swim? And in the face of this unlikely outcome of a recently established behavior pattern, who would dare to predict to what the new-found ability to swim might lead?

Yet strangely enough—or perhaps not so surprisingly, in view of the basic uniformity of mankind's cerebral mechanisms—no matter where, geographically, nor when, over the entire span of human history, local cultures have developed into advanced civilizations, we find that mankind's greatest thoughts, as epitomized in the writings of philosophers, show remarkable similarity. Some views on the nature and value of knowledge, intellect, and intelligence propounded by wise men of China, so distant from us in place and time, are stunning in their modernity and still current validity.

In the misty beginnings of China's long cultural history, Lao-tze, expounding his concept of Tao, the Way (of nature and of wise living), is reported to have insisted that knowledge is not virtue and neither is it wisdom, for nothing is so far from a sage as an "intellectual." The worst government would be one of philosophers, he is said to have averred, for they botch every natural process with theory; their ability to make speeches and multiply ideas is precisely the sign of their incapacity for action!

At a later period the philosopher Chuang-tze, who lived about 370 B.C., showed a sophistication of thinking that we find difficult to credit to those early times. He wrote that problems are due less to the nature of things than to the limits of our thought; that it is not to be wondered at that the effort of our imprisoned brains to understand the cosmos of which they are such minute particles should end in contradiction.

He spoke of the limits of intellect; the attempt to explain the whole in terms of the part has been a gigantic immodesty, forgivable only on the ground of the amusement it has caused, for humor, like philosophy, is a view of the part in terms of the whole, and neither is possible without the other. The intellect, said Chuang-tze, can never avail to understand ultimate things, or any profound thing, such as the growth of a child. In order to understand the Tao, one must "sternly suppress one's knowledge": we have to suppress our theories and feel fact. Education is of no help toward such understanding: submission in the flow of nature is all-important.

And Wang Yang-ming, who lived from 1472 to 1528, practically summarized our present thesis when he wrote: "The mind itself is the embodiment of natural law. Is there anything in the universe that exists independent of mind? Is there any law apart from the mind?"

Experts feel certain that the human brain has not undergone any significant biological change since the time of the Neanderthals, who, as evidenced in the excavations at the Shanidar caves high in the mountains of Kurdistan in northern Iraq, built fires, cared for their sick, conducted funeral rites, and put flowers with the bodies of their dead. In the last twenty to thirty thousand years, we know from archaeological findings of both historic and prehistoric periods, a high degree of intellectual accomplishment has been uniformly present in all the branches of our species throughout its existence. The brief excerpts we have given of ancient Chinese thought about thought would certainly seem to bear out this opinion.

There is, however, an outcome of the cumulative nature of culture that we must not overlook. Increasingly, as intelligence adapts a living creature to its environment by the use of artifacts and through the cultural transmission of knowledge, the creature and the environment modify each other at an exponential rate. Perfect adaptation, of the order of the ants' or the termites', which has existed in a balance between being and habitat unchanged over hundreds of millions of years, is not possible for mankind. The rapidity of change in our cultural habitat presents a perpetual and continuing stimulus to bodily and, above all, to mental adaptation, which of necessity increases the demands on the new brain to devise accommodation, and in the process speeds both the rate of change and the need for new changes.

And so, although we recognize that we have been mistaken in thinking that the technological advances of Western man might indicate new departures in the human brain's capacities, yet we have also to recognize that that technology is rapidly creating a totally new environment for our species. This new environment may well ultimately affect our species' future development; precisely this technology may prove to be the turning point through which, in negotiating it, we may find ourselves in a process of extinction as Homo sapiens and in a stage of transition toward Homo neocorticus.
 
There are, of course, far too many imponderables involved to feel confident in predicting the future course of our species. Among these are the course of technology itself and how far from natural processes it can carry us before it becomes subject to its own limitations. There is the matter of human population density and whether it will be adjusted by natural means or can be adapted to the biosphere that is our habitat by cultural or social means. There is the question of the medical preservation of the "unfit" and whether we can remain viable at all as a species with the increasing maladaptive dilution of our gene pools. And there is the possibility that ecological interference may ultimately make mankind's existence untenable.

Our evolutionary development may be reaching the end of a line for biological if not for cultural reasons, but we ourselves are inclined to discount this. We believe subtle biological factors to be operating that are not yet clearly discernible, but which may be recognizable in retrospect. Another factor as simple and probable as the advent of another ice age, for instance, would effectively alter and reequilibrate the balance between man and nature, and must also be kept in mind as a possibility.

We believe that the possibility and even the likelihood remain for a true evolutionary progression in the anatomical and physiological configuration of the brain, much like the progression that occurred between apes and man. In that case the departure would be just as radical, and it would have as a consequence new behavioral response patterns that at this point we cannot visualize and about which we can only speculate.

To assume that this new superintelligence would occupy itself with creating a new, weird, and wonderful technology is a naive exercise of human fantasy. Ultimately technology exists to serve the greater comfort of individuals, and a superintelligence may well find other means of achieving this end. Thus, were such a superintelligence to be found in some other planetary system, we might be confronted with something totally alien to our understanding, and even to our imagination.

It seems to us that we can predict as a next development the extension of an actual and currently observable trend. We see in mankind the persistence of a brain function that has lost its original purpose. This function is seated in the brain's so-called limbic system—the areas of the brain that govern our emotions. This still active part of our brain mediates all those built-in and semiautomatic responses that enhanced an ability to survive in our remote animal past. It achieves those responses by setting up in the animal (and, vestigially, in us) an emotional state that promotes the performance of the necessary actions. It is an inherent property of animal behavior patterns that they are coupled with governing emotions.

As human beings, we tend to regard our own emotions, coupled with our social propensities, as a particularly high and noble facet of our behavior, and it may well be that this is so, but we have to assume that an animal has far more specific feelings and passions than are known by humans, since a generalized "mood" must precede any action it takes. Konrad Lorenz has quoted Heinroth as saying, "Animals are emotional people of low intelligence," and he himself observed, "We should assume that the realm of human feelings must exhibit a similar process of simplification and abolition of differentiation to that known to have occurred in human instinctive behavior."

The limbic area of our brain still regulates our emotions, and so it is, in effect, the seat of our "humanity." Because of it we feel parental and sexual feelings, know pleasure or distress, fear or anger, start at strange sounds, dislike unfamiliar sights and smells, experience love and hate and the entire complex of emotions of which human beings are capable. It is perfectly apparent that the limbic system is still very much in operation, and yet we nevertheless find equally apparent a growing tendency for the newest part of our brain, the neocortex, to overtake and usurp many of its functions.

The neocortex is the seat of our reasoning abilities. It mediates our capacity for judgment, for making decisions based on logical thought, for solving problems, for evaluation and choice. In human beings there is an increasing propensity for neocortical reason to impinge upon biologically older limbic emotions in determining our ultimate behavior. Indeed, there is hardly an area of human function that is not now, at least to some extent, subject to neocortical control.

If we feel a surge of anger, we are able, if we will, to stay it and to question whether it is a justifiable response. It goes so far that such basic functions as sex and reproduction may be modified by our reason. Even as we feel a biological attraction to an opposite-sex person, our reason is questioning: Will he or she enjoy the things we enjoy? Will he fit into our social scheme? Will she be as interesting a companion as she is good to look at? Whether or not we follow up that initial attraction with invitation or acceptance of further acquaintanceship is often determined by our answers to those promptings of reason.

To dramatize and highlight the coexistence of the two conflicting bases of contemporary human brain function, we have only to look at our scientific community. On the one hand, those engaged in the so-called pure sciences, like mathematics, physics, or astronomy, rigidly exclude emotional involvement and stress logic in the conclusions they arrive at and in every aspect of their work. On the other hand, practitioners of the psychological sciences, especially those concerned with mental health, place inordinate importance on "freeing" the emotions from the shackles of the reasoning mind, in the belief that the reasoning mind is the culprit and chief cause of mankind's maladjustments.

The dual and often conflicting mental motivations indeed reach as far as the ultimate purpose to which our natural life is geared—the production, sustaining, and rearing of a new generation, which today is also subjected to the processes of reason. We ask ourselves such questions as how many children we can afford to raise and, indeed, whether we wish to have children at all. No longer do our emotions alone mediate even the continuity of our species.

Of course, in the vast diversity of human beings, there are many who still are governed in their actions more by their emotions than by their reason; those who are more neocortically controlled are still a minority among us. There is a whole spectrum between these extremes, but no aspect of the old brain's function except the purely autonomic is not to some extent influenced and in the process of becoming dominated by the new.

The most likely next phase of intelligence in our own species, then, is toward the total control of our emotions by the neocortex, that is, by judgment, reason, and logic. Such a process would eventually eliminate entirely such responses as love, hate, maternal feelings, anxiety, and also many of the derivative mental consequences of those emotions, such as elation, joy, enthusiasm, as well as depression, psychosomatic diseases, and so on. With this development, should it materialize, everything that makes us what we are would disappear and our descendants would appear to us to be feelingless automatons. They, on the other hand, from their vantage point in time, would look upon us as primitive forerunners of their species, much as we regard apes and monkeys.

Naturally, in saying that the neocortex would eliminate our basic emotions, this does not imply that all emotion would necessarily be expunged from the repertoire of our posterity. The brain has pleasure centers, and these, if denied the stimulus of the old emotions, would surely find new material in purely neocortical responses. The brain's pleasure centers might then find new function in elaborating such neocortical pleasures as aesthetic appreciation, scientific research and theorizing, the special joys of puzzle solving, and similar satisfactions of the judgmental and reasoning mind.

It could well be that when the neocortex finally takes over completely and is instrumental in producing our hypothetical new species, they will be better adapted to life than we—freed, as they would be, from mankind's unabating internal dissonance between feelings and reason. The apes are admirably adapted to the circumstances of their lives by the operation of their limbic system and with but little demand upon the resources of the budding neocortex of which they are possessed; this future race will equally experience the benefits of having only one dominant cerebral function—in their case, the reasoning neocortex. The whole course of human history may prove to have been but a comparatively short stage of shifting between these two states of balance.

All other species have no option but to be at ease in the niche of life in which they find themselves: the ape does not question its reason for being or its destiny—it simply lives—and the future race will be unlikely to experience such mixed feelings as we know. In the final count, Homo sapiens may be distinguished not for "brain" and "culture," but as the creature that knew dissatisfaction, that lived in "divine discontent," precisely because, in our line, the emerging neocortex has reached in us a stage where it challenges and to some extent controls the old brain, but does not yet have the field to itself. We are in our minds and in our whole being basically dual, and this, beyond any of our technical achievements, is the mark of Homo sapiens.

Whether such a line of development as this occurs in other parts of the universe, where higher intelligences have evolved, is open to speculation. It could be that it is a development that is idiosyncratic to ourselves, but we are not inclined to postulate unique phenomena, and we think it more likely that a stage similar to ours is inevitable in the development of any very high intelligence anywhere. Transitional phases are unavoidable in any line of evolution, and so we believe we should expect to find stages like ours wherever higher life forms and intelligences emerge.

Any transitional stage, of its nature, is not very stable and record does not provide extensive evidence of linking forms. Hence, the long search for the transitional forms—the so-called missing link—that bridged the gap between the apes and ourselves, a gap that is only now being filled in by very recent discoveries in Africa of the australopithecines and other linking forms.

If, however, we look back at the whole line of primate development less anthropocentrically, we or our descendants may yet find that the australopithecines were a comparatively stable form when compared with us, Homo sapiens. The future may well reveal that we, along with Neanderthal, Cromagnon, and other comparatively recent forerunners, were each and all of us "trial runs" in nature's progress toward a more stable form to house high intelligence. The very fact that we find in ourselves competing mental systems within one brain should alert us to the probability that we ourselves are a kind of "missing link" between the apes and an eventual, more harmoniously adapted Homo neocorticus.

This, of course, poses tremendous problems in planning to establish interplanetary communications. Perhaps it is only in the transitional and transitory stage peculiar to our species —and at that, only for a short phase in the course of its development—that any feeling of need exists at all to reach out and explore. It is a propensity that has marked our progress. Where other creatures have found circumscribed habitats and have adapted to them, mankind has found challenge in new horizons and constantly searched to find them, whether in papyrus-reed craft, balsa-wood rafts, sailing boats, airplanes, or interplanetary rockets.

Now that we begin to understand the vastness of the universe, we feel lonely and isolated on our speck of a planet in one far corner of it. But for interplanetary communication to eventuate, we probably have to seek the long odds that another world has produced another species that reaches our own stage at about the same time as we, or rather at the time that would allow for the journey's distance to reach them. It is not impossible—indeed, it is probable—that in the vastness of the universe such worlds exist.

And where do we go from here? What kind of brain will supersede ours?
The superbrain of Homo neocorticus, as we have said, need not be markedly larger than ours in order to produce a mind of a completely other quality. It must, however, be somewhat bigger, both in the number of its neurons and in the axon and dendrite connections between them—and therefore to a certain extent in absolute mass—to achieve that change.

From the ready-made laboratory we have available on earth, we can see in the progression from an apelike brain to the human one, first, a fuller use of the potentialities of the smaller brain. The australopithecines—the so-called man-apes—for example, retained brain dimensions no greater than those of modern apes, yet they fabricated primitive tools and took the first steps toward a human way of life over long ages, before environmental circumstances put a premium on still greater intelligence. Only then did larger-brained species emerge. Among ourselves, in our own species, we are aware of a notable difference in brain performance all the way from the dull, to the average, to the very bright, and up to the genius, while all these brains are objectively of the same dimensions and potentially all capable of performance equal to that of the genius.

It may be illuminating to look at another organ, or organ system, rather than at the brain, to realize how far one can go in utilizing what already exists before it becomes necessary to increase size for additional performance. The muscles of a 90-pound weakling, for instance, are physically no different at birth from those of a 200-pound weight lifter, and they have the same potential for development. The eventual difference lies in the fact that the weight lifter has exploited his muscle power to its fullest possibilities, while the weakling has failed to do so.

In this respect, the brain is no different from any other organ. Its constant use and exercise fosters better performance at a progressive rate. And so we must visualize circumstances in which the demands made upon human brainpower are such as to foster the full use of the tissue already at our disposal—in fact, a time when what we now think of as "very bright" or "genius" performance becomes "average"—before we have to contemplate any increase in size to facilitate greater powers. If we can imagine a society in which what we now think of as genius becomes dull-to-normal intellectual performance, and try to visualize the geniuses of that society, we then catch a glimpse of the first step toward Homo neocorticus.

We can also get a second glimpse of that distant creature in our contemporary earth-laboratory through another of the existing trends we have mentioned: that the area of increased development and growth of brain will surely continue the present tendency of the expansion of neocortical function at the expense of the limbic system. After all, what is our present image of a "civilized" person as opposed to a "wild" one? It is of a person who has control of his emotions, one who exercises reason and judgment, and who does not get "carried away" by personal passions. Again, if we can visualize a society in which the behavior of the most highly "civilized" of us has become the norm and in which a new, far more highly civilized elect has begun to make an appearance, we may then fill in a second color in our picture of the future of intelligence.

We may project that the brain stem—that part of our cerebral accoutrement that governs the autonomic functions of the body (such as breathing, circulation, and digestion)—may well take over some of the currently more primitive response patterns of the limbic region, while the neocortex absorbs its higher "emotional" functions. Thus, Homo neocorticus would arrive at a truly dominant neocortex—a condition that is now only partly the case, but that may be seen as en route in Homo sapiens.

Given such a brain, let us take a closer look at it. Its enormous capacity to integrate information must eventually make our conventional languages almost obsolete. Our words and sentence structure would become too cumbersome to express the increased speed of the flow of thoughts and too imprecise to frame the increased refinement of perception.

We do not think that language of our present type would ever become totally obsolete, because we believe it to be a strong element in mother-child attachment and bonding, and therefore a stage through which the future infant would have to pass, but we do foresee it becoming less useful as that child gets older. We can imagine scientists of the future studying their infants' language as a clue to their understanding of the psychological processes of their distant predecessors, much as we study embryology to confirm our ideas about anatomical evolution.

We imagine that in that future time, adult communication will be effected by a new type of "language," one much more condensed than ours, and also more precise—perhaps something along the lines of our present mathematical symbolism, extended and made more versatile. After all, our present brain—that is, the brain of Homo sapiens—evolved to meet the needs of our species over the ages before the time of written language. One of its especially important functions in our development was its ability to "package" knowledge in a form that could be stored, and thus enable our forerunners to pass on the experience of their lifetimes to each rising generation. Today, the sheer quantity of information available to us has become so vast that it is quite impossible for any single mind to encompass all of it in the space of a lifetime, either in its extent or in its precision. We imagine that the Homo neocorticus brain will be able to do this.

This means that their increased brain capacity will be put to work encompassing a body of knowledge that increases geometrically within each generation. This will probably not be achieved by the addition of larger storage capacity, for huge increases in storage capacity must eventually become cumbersome and in any case limited by physical factors. Rather, we think, it will be made possible by an increase in the ability to digest far larger quantities of data simultaneously and to deduce solutions in an instant.

We now have to commit large bodies of information to memory to see their connections and to match them with experience in order to utilize them appropriately, whether in conversation or in social endeavor. Homo neocorticus, on the other hand, will probably be able to rediscover over and over again, without effort, all that the combined efforts of countless scientific minds of our type have cumulatively been able to produce over lifetimes. They will not have to learn and commit to memory, say, a theory of relativity, but would see the principle clearly and immediately, and have the means to be able to express it, any and every time it might be needed. In fact, the concept would be as self-evident to them as the fact that a rose is a rose is to us.

All these things we can see to some extent from the ways in which our own brains are tending. But even in this we must be cautious. Apes, for instance, when they are tested in laboratories, show a great reserve of intelligence and ability that they are never called upon to exploit in their natural habitats. They have also shown remarkable adaptability when experimentally transferred by scientists to other natural habitats greatly different from their own.

In one such experiment, observers removed five chimpanzees to a desolate island in a lake in the northwest of Russia—an area that they described as a "cool jungle." Although night temperatures there often dropped as low as 10 degrees above freezing, the observers reported they adapted so well that "they might as well have been back home in Africa." They made cozy shelters out of branches resembling armchairs and hammocks, reported Leonid Firsov, director of the anthropoid center of the Academy of Sciences of USSR, I. P. Pavlov Institute of Physiology in Leningrad. The temperature of their "beds," warmed by their bodies, reached about 97° F. They found half of the 180 varieties of plants edible, and they dined on leaves, seeds, mushrooms, ants, and dragonflies. Even more fascinating, experiments showed that the chimpanzees skillfully used sticks, branches, and stones as working implements, and that they had a rich memory of association.

An ape could never know which of its own potentialities would prove useful and become exponentially developed in a higher form. Similarly, we ourselves can hardly know what circumstances will bring a more powerful new brain into being, nor what purposes it will eventually serve. We can only be aware of the enormous reserve capacity that resides in us, and speculate about its direction.

We take it as an axiom that an organ seldom develops, and never persists, unless a function is or becomes apparent for which it can be used. At that point we know that an organ will take on any function that it can perform, and that its use will improve its performance. Indeed, this reserve power that seems to lie dormant in some species until environment provokes its potential into actual use, has been called by anthropologists preadaptation. In the primate line, it was the dormant potential of the simian brain that permitted the evolution of the human.

Nevertheless, since we like to speculate, we may fairly entertain the thought that perhaps some of the extreme intellectual feats that are now being accomplished by humans may in turn reach their apotheosis in a descendant species. What seems to us now to be a comparatively useless predilection for, say, philosophy, or for so-called pure science, may eventually turn out for them to be the basis of an entirely new way of life, and in that context they may well think of it as a preadaptation. Thus, a genius of today may turn out to be an advance guard, or the embodiment of a preadaptation that is even now paving the way to future Homo neocorticus. Among that future species, what we now consider as the highest, and even unnecessary, levels of mental activity will be commonplace.

In this context it is not without interest that in our time, too, there is a cultural practice that certainly must have an effect of reinforcing by sexual selection the trends that we discern as being brought about by natural selection. The practice is unicontinue their general education through school and on into universities, advanced technical training centers, and other institutions of higher learning.

In these places those of our young people who have shown superior mental abilities are brought together and segregated for several years just at that period of their lives when they are physically and emotionally ready for breeding, so that the natural outcome of this social practice is to foster mating between individuals who to a certain extent have been preselected for their intelligence. Over comparatively few generations this inbreeding of our more gifted young people must have some effect on the mental abilities shown in the succeeding generations of their offspring. Thus, two powerful forces, natural selection (by which the more adaptively endowed survive in greater proportion whenever competition between breeding groups arises) and sexual selection (by which males and females in their choice of mates tend thereby to fix in the opposite sex those traits that they find desirable and attractive), in our species and in our time, give indications of being mutually reinforcing.

Is this as far as we can extrapolate from what we know of Laboratory Earth in our attempts to see into our own future or into other worlds?

Actually, it is not. It is within the realm of our reason and imagination to catch glimpses of still more distant possible descendants.

We see that in nature successful solutions often arise by what is called convergence—that is, they arise independently in different lines that are often separated in time and place and are in no way related to each other, because these lines have encountered similar environmental problems and have coped with them in a similar way precisely because that solution is an efficient one. Prehensile tails, for example, have developed in many kinds of monkey (although not in all), in the common opposum, and in several related species, in the true chameleon and a few lesser genera, and in a fish, the sea horse. Similar needs in these disparate creatures have evoked a similar response in the structure and function of the tail.

In earlier pages we have from time to time described what amounts to a group brain as it exists among earth's social insects, which produces intelligent solutions, although the individuals comprising the groups are separately only capable of comparatively stereotyped actions. What if a group brain were to evolve where all the individuals were possessed of the kind of superbrain and superintelligence we are attributing to Homo neocorticus?

Utilizing the concept of convergence, we see that the social insects, by the use of chemical substances—the pheromones—that give off distinctive odors, have achieved almost instant communication, an important element in their cooperative actions. In a mass brain of cooperative superbrains that might evolve from Homo neocorticus, we could imagine an absolutely new type of communication emerging, no longer either verbal or even more refinedly symbolical. It could be a direct brain-to-brain awareness promoted by an individual brain's electric charges that might be intensified or modified so as to constitute an instant transmission without any kind of language, receptor mechanisms being evolved within the brain itself.

In order to conceptualize this thought, we paid a visit to a modern laboratory where electroencephalograms of patients being tested for brain disturbances are being recorded and processed all day long. We saw there a technician in her middle years who had spent all her working life recording and interpreting the "squiggles" registered by inked points onto long rolls of papers as they responded finely to the electric emanations of the patients' brains. The lengths of paper were subsequently folded into bound volumes.

This technician could open any of those records and read them, understanding all they implied, as easily as we read any page printed in our own language, and she was teaching her young assistants to do the same. In other words, the direct electric emanations from half a dozen different areas of the patient's brain simultaneously, with their respective periods of excitement and calm, had clear meaning for her, and she could read like a book any of the volumes in the large library of records of brain activity that were assembled on the shelves of her office.

If we can imagine these inked lines of the EEC squiggles as an extremely primitive and crude form of what might ultimately evolve as a form of direct brain-to-brain communication, we believe we have a plausible model on which to base our speculations about our descendant species-after-next. Such instantaneous communication could easily suffuse an entire group and make possible intelligent mass action of an order that we can no longer imagine, or even speculate about.

The question that remains is: Why such a brain? What possible environmental circumstance could ever evoke it?

And the answer is that once a higher intelligence appears on the natural scene, it makes its own environment. It is a matter of common observation among ourselves that every human being leaves a mark of his or her own personality upon surroundings. A housewife's individuality can almost be read by the condition in which we find her house—the order or disorder in which it is kept, the way it is furnished and decorated, the implements and equipment she uses. A man's fields or garden, office or workshop or den, also reveal much about him.

It would seem that something of the brain and perhaps of the whole nervous system of each of us is displayed in the environments we make for ourselves. Even the little bowerbird reveals his individuality in the small objects he uses to decorate his bower and in his arrangement of them. And so we feel we are justified in assuming that as a brain evolves in complexity, it will surely be to the accompaniment of an increasingly complex ambience.

Human beings originally lived in the same "nature" as all other animals, and many still live very closely with it. If we camp in a forest, we do not actually need our superior mental equipment—that other creatures live out their lives in that forest is evidence of this. But most of us no longer live in natural settings, and even those who do, modify them with such artifacts as fire, weapons, and tools.

Today, all of mankind, some to a smaller but most to a larger extent, make our own habitats, and our man-made environment becomes more and more complex. Moreover, in the process of adjusting to our increasingly artificial surroundings, we become less and less fitted to live in natural ones, and so the course upon which we are embarked is far more likely to continue than to be reversed. For Homo neocorticus we can project a habitat of, to us, incredible complexity, and we can imagine that this will provide the spur that could eventually produce the Group-brained neocorticus, whom we could no longer designate as Homo.

At this point we pass our project over to our reader to use his or her imagination to visualize a meeting between Homo sapiens and Group-brained neocorticus, or its prototype as it may exist somewhere in the vastness of the universe.
 
 
That is excerpted from "Beyond Human Intelligence" Chapter 11, Other Senses, Other Worlds, 1976. Doris & David Jonas. It has been out of print since 1989 when the publisher went bankrupt and closed its doors. I read it in 1976 via a “new nonfiction book release” copy from my local library (and I have subsequently acquired a hardbound copy). It has aged quite well topically.
 
A book cover with a face and text

AI-generated content may be incorrect.
 
BACK TO THE BLIND SPOT
 
From The Afterword